SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.11 número2-3Notas sobre as Representações Sociais e o Habitu: Esboço de uma análise comparadaThe role of group status and history of the conflict on intergroup discrimination strategies índice de autoresíndice de assuntosPesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Journal

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Não possue artigos similaresSimilares em SciELO

Compartilhar


Psicologia

versão impressa ISSN 0874-2049

Psicologia vol.11 no.2-3 Lisboa jun. 1996

https://doi.org/10.17575/rpsicol.v11i2/3.608 

Spontaneous activation of schemata under stress: getting control over eathquake threat

Luísa Lima*

*ISCTE, Lisboa

 


ABSTRACT

Beliefs of personal control over uncontrollable events have often been found to be related to a sense of well being (Taylor & Brown, 1988). In this paper we argue that, under threat and when personal control is impossible, subjects will automatically activate other types of control beliefs (secondary control beliefs) to cope with the threat. In three studies, earthquake threat and the feeling of personal uncontrollability were made salient in the experimental condition, and the RT to secondary-related control words were assessed. Results show that two types of secondary control beliefs were activated under threat: religious control and political control. This result shows that these more social types of beliefs are used to cope with threat when more personal beliefs are not available.


RESUMO

A percepção de controlo pessoal sobre acontecimentos incontroláveis tem sido associada à procura de um sentimento de bem-estar psicológico (Taylor & Brown, 1988). Neste artigo procuramos mostrar que, quando os sujeitos estão sob ameaça e o controlo pessoal é impossível, os sujeitos activam automaticamente outros tipos de crenças de controlo (as crenças de controlo secundário) de modo a conseguirem lidar com a ameaça. Nos três estudos que apresentamos, tornámos saliente a ameaça sísmica e a ausência de controlo pessoal sobre a ameaça (condição experimental) e o tempo de reacção a palavras associadas ao controlo secundário foi avaliado. Os resultados mostram que há dois tipos de crenças de controlo automaticamente activadas em condição de ameaça: o controlo religioso e o controlo político. Estes resultados mostram que estes tipos de crenças mais sociais são utilizadas para lidar com situações de ameaça quando as crenças pessoais não estão disponíveis.


 

1. Problem and Theoretical Approach.

The idea that a sense of control or mastery is an essential part of psychological wellbeing has been widely accepted. But research has repeatedly found that perceptions of control go far beyond our actual capabilities to master the environment, and they are extended to situations were no objective control is exerted (e.g., Langer, 1975). Contrarily to objectivistic views of mental health, beliefs of control over uncontrollable events have been found to positively relate to wellbeing (Taylor, 1983; Taylor & Brown, 1988). Within this kind of approach, coping with earthquake threat is an interesting topic to study because it deals with coping with a highly destructive and uncontrollable event. As a matter of fact:

1. Earthquake hazard is the most threatening natural hazard that Portuguese population has to face.

2. Although seismic phenomena are well characterised by the geophysical Sciences, it is still impossible to accurately predict the occurrence of such a destructive eveíit.

3. According to our previous research, people perceive seismic risk both as a highly uncontrollable risk and a risk that affects themseives. Nevertheless, other studies on the Portuguese population show that the common citizen is not informed about what to do in case of an earthquake.

In Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) model of stress, the possibility of an earthquake, being perceived as personally threatening (primary appraisal) and uncontrollable (secondary appraisal), corresponds to a «stressor», leading to psychological and physiological stress responses. As there is nothing to do to prevent the earthquake to happen, according to the same model people will engage in emotion focused coping strategies to deal with the unpleasant feelings the threat imposes on them.

Research on systematic exposition to hazards has shown that people who live in hazard prone environments develop some cognitive adjustments to deal with danger and uncertainty (e.g., Kates, Burton and White, 1978). In line with those results, we have shown in a field study (Lima, 1991) that people who move to an environment with higher levels of seismic risk present stronger control beliefs over the earthquakes than those who remained in lower seismic risk areas. Those control beliefs are a kind of vicarious secondary control (Rothbaum et. al, 1982), whereby people rely on some more powerful agent to restore their sense of control over the environment, when they cannot control it themselves. In our research, although subjects perceived earthquakes as externally determined, they could still keep some control over the phenomenon, using three types of socially produced control beliefs:

Religious Control: If God is considered as the ultimate determiner of earthquakes, believers can start a series of actions in order to influence God’s Will (to pray, to make a promise) and to prevent a disaster.

Scientific-Technical Control: If earthquakes are considered as natural phenomena and Science is seen as efficient to deal with nature, we can suppose that the confidence on Science and technology is another form of restoring the control over an uncertain situation.

Political Control: If there is poli tical trust, if people believe that their government has efficient plans to deal with a disaster, they can restore their sense of mastering over the environment.

In line with those results, this póster presents an experimental study that tries to show that: When earthquake threat and the impossibility ofpersonal control is made salient, people will spontaneously activate control schemata to cope with the threat. This hypothesis will be tested in three studies, each one dealing with a specific kind of control beliefs: Religious, Scientific and Political.

 

Figure 1. Graphical representation of the theoretical model used

UNREADABLE IMAGE

 

Method

Subjects

480 students participated in these studies, which ran on two phases. On the First Phase, all subjects answered a questionnaire containing a control over the earthquake beliefs scale (Lima, 1991, see Table 1) and a trait anxiety scale (STAIY2, Spielberger et al., 1983). The second part of the experiment took place a month later. On this Second Phase 79 subjects (St1:N=24; St2:N=28; St3: N=27) were selected, according to: (i) their anxiety levels: low trait anxiety subjects; and (ii) strength of control beliefs: either very high (schematic subjects) or very low aschematic subjects) on each of the three types of control beliefs).

Manipulated variable:

The independent manipulated variable in this study was «earthquake threat». In each of the three studies, subjects were randomly assigned to either the experimental or the control conditions.

In the experimental condition (Threat) subjects were told that they would participate in a study concerning the responses to an emergency situation. They were trained in the experimental task until they fnlly understood it, and they were asked to watch a 3 minutes video film about earthquakes. This film had been previously tested to induce a sense of threat, and the perception of low possibilities of personal control or forecast over the earthquake threat. Immediately afterwards, the experimental task took place.

In the control conditions, subjects rehearsed the experimental task and afterwards they performed it.

 

 

Experimental task:

The task employed was a word location test (Cambor et al., 1988; Stward et al., 1991). Subjects were asked to press a key (left or right) as quickly as possible in response to the location of a target word. The target word was presented either above or bellow a fixation point, indicated by

«------x------»

in the centre of a Computer monitor. Target words were presented for 500 mseconds, with a 1.5 seconds interval between trials. Reaction times (RTs) were recorded. Each subject practised on a series of 10 neutral words. The experimental task consisted of 52 words, including 26 schema related words (i.e., «God» for religion schematic subjects; «engineering» for the Science schematic subjects and «government» for the politic schematic subjects) and 26 neutral words paired on length and frequency with the others.

Results

Study 1 - Religious schem.

A three way ANOVA (with repeated measures on the last factor) was performed on the mean RTs : Experimental condition (control vs. threat) x Group (schematic vs. aschematic) x Valence (schema related word vs. neutral word). Results show that all subjects respond quicker to neutral than religious words, except schematic subjects under threat. In that situation, religious subjects respond quicker to the schema related words than to the neutral words (Table 2 and Figure 2). This result, compatible with our predictions, allows us to say that in a condition of earthquake threat, religious subjects spontaneously activate their control beliefs in order to restore their sense of control over the environment. Separate ANOVAS performed on neutral and schema related words confirm this result: while there is no significam effect for the neutral words (FExp.cond(1,20)=.088, p>.70; FGroup(1,20)=.883, p>.30; FInteraction(1,20)=378, p>.70), there is a significant interaction in the case of religious words (FInteraction(1,20)=5.44, p<.03; FExp.cond(1,20)=A22, p>.50; FGroup(1,20)=.895, p>30).

 

 

Figure 2. Study 1: means profile

UNREADABLE IMAGE

 

Study 2 - Scientific schema

A three way ANOVA (with a repeated measure on the last factor) was performed on the mean RTs : Experimental condition (control vs. threat) x Group (schematic vs. aschematic) x Valence (schema related word vs. neutral word). Contrarily to our predictions, no significant effects were found. Separate ANOVAs performed on neutral and schema related words do not show any significant effects either (Table 3 and Figure 3).

 

 

Figure 3. Study 2: means profile

UNREADABLE IMAGE

 

Study 3 - Political schema

The same kind of ANOVA was performed on RTs related to this kind of schema. The pattern of results is very similar to the first study, though the results only approach the significance level. Separate 2 (Group) x 2 (Experimental Condition) ANOVAS were performed for each type of word (Table 4 and Figure 4).

 

 

Figure 4. Study 3: means profile

UNREADABLE IMAGE

 

For schema related words, only the interaction effect approaches the significance levels (FInteraction(1,23)=2.97, p<.09; FExp.cond(1,23)=.237, p>.60; FGroup(1,23)=37, p>.50). For neutral words there are no significant effects (FExp.cond.(1,23)=.055, p>.80; FGroup(1,23)=1.065, p>30; FInteraction (1,23)=.233, p>.60).

These results tend to show that only schematic subjects under stress preferentially process schema related words, in order to regain the sense of control over the threat.

Conclusions

Our results show that two kinds of control schemata are activated by subjects in an earthquake threat condition: religious and poli tical control schemata. We could find no evidence for the activation of the scientific schema in the same situation

These kind of control schemata, as we argued elsewhere (Lima, 1990), are cognitive products of interactive social groups, who, for a long time, have managed to cope with stress in a earthquake prone area. The individual activation of these kind of cognitive structure in a threat situation is, on our view, an example of both the impact of social factors on individual cognition and the motivationally driven cognitive processing.

 

References

Cambor, R., Shear, M. K., Speilman, L., Bargh, J., & Sweeney, J. (1988, May). Attentional bias in panic disorder.         [ Links ] Communication presented at the 141 th Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Ássociation, Montreal, Canada.

Kates, R. W., Burton, I., & White, G. F. (1978). The environment as hazard. New York: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311-328.         [ Links ]

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal and Coping. New York: Springer.         [ Links ]

Lima, M. L. (1990). Controlar o incontrolável: esquemas cognitivos de adaptação ao risco sísmico. Revista de Psicologia Social, 5(2-3), 171-183.         [ Links ]

Lima, M. L. (1991, July). Cognitive schemata to cope with seismic risk: moving to a high risk environment.         [ Links ] Póster presented to the International Conference Stress, Anxiety and Emotional Disorders. Braga, Portugal.

Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self: a two process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5-37.         [ Links ]

Steward, S. H., Achille, M., Dubois-Nguyen, I., & Phill, R. O. (1991, July). Sellective attention bias for threat in anxiety sensitive subjects.         [ Links ] Póster presented to the International Conference Stress, Anxiety and Emotional Disorders. Braga, Portugal.

Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38, 1161-1173.         [ Links ]

Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well being: A social psychology perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103,193-210.         [ Links ]

Creative Commons License Todo o conteúdo deste periódico, exceto onde está identificado, está licenciado sob uma Licença Creative Commons